53. 2018-06-02 《Memory》:Mental time travel

作者 简单的镜子
转载请标明原作者和出处
也可以关注我的简书

TIME’S ARROW IS BENT INTO A LOOP

   Memory was one of the first fields of study for psychologists in the 19th century, as it was closely connected with the concept of consciousness, which had formed the bridge between philosophy and psychology. Hermann Ebbinghaus in particular devoted much of his research to the scientific study of memory and learning, but the next generation of psychologists turned their attention to a behaviourist study of learning, and “conditioning” replaced memory as the focus of research.

   Apart from a few isolated studies, notably by Bluma Zeigarnik and Frederic Bartlett in the 1920s and 30s, memory was largely ignored as a topic until the “cognitive revolution” took place following World War II. Cognitive psychologists began to explore the idea of the brain as an information processor, and this provided a model for the storage of memory: it was seen as a process, whereby some items passed from short-term or working memory into long-term memory.

image.png

   By the time Endel Tulving finished his doctorate in 1957, memory was once more a central area of study. Forced to abandon the study of visual perception due to a lack of facilities, Tulving turned his attention to memory. The funding deficit also shaped his approach to the subject, designing experiments that used no more than a pen, some paper, and a supply of index cards.

"Relating what we know about the behaviour of memory to the underlying neural structures is not at all obvious. That’s real science."
                        Endel Tulving

The free-recall method

   Learning about the subject as he went along, Tulving worked in a rather unorthodox way, which occasionally earned him criticism from his peers, and was to make publishing his results difficult. His maverick instincts did, however, lead to some truly innovative research. One hurriedly designed, ad hoc demonstration to a class of students in the early 1960s was to provide him with the model for many later experiments.

   He read out a random list of 20 everyday words to the students, and then asked them to write down as many as they could recall, in any order. As he expected, most of them managed to remember around half of the list. He then asked them about the words that they had not remembered, giving hints such as “Wasn’t there a colour on the list?”, after which the student could often provide the correct answer.

   Tulving developed a series of experiments on this “free recall” method, during which he noticed that people tend to group words together into meaningful categories; the better they organize the information, the better they are able to remember it. His subjects were also able to recall a word when given a cue in the form of the category (such as “animals”) in which they had mentally filed that word.

   Tulving concluded that although all the words memorized from the list were actually available for remembering, the ones that were organized by subject were more readily accessible to memory, especially when the appropriate cue was given. In Tulving’s free recall experiments, people were asked to remember as many words as possible from a random list. “Forgotten” words were often recalled using category cues. They were stored in memory but temporarily inaccessible.

Memory types

   Where previous psychologists had concentrated on the process of storing information, and the failings of that process, Tulving made a distinction between two different processes – storage and retrieval of information – and showed how the two were linked.In the course of his research, Tulving was struck by the fact that there seemed to be different kinds of memory.

   The distinction between long-term memory and short-term memory had already been established, but Tulving felt there was more than one kind of long-term memory. He saw a difference between memories that are knowledge-based (facts and data), and those that are experience-based (events and conversations).

   He proposed a division of long-term memory into two distinct types: semantic memory, the store of facts; and episodic memory, the repository of our personal history and events. Tulving’s experiments had demonstrated that organization of semantic information, such as lists of words, helps efficient recollection, and the same appeared to be true of episodic memory. But where semantic memories are organized into meaningful categories of subject matter, episodic memories are organized by relation to the specific time or circumstances in which they were originally stored.

   For example, a particular conversation may have taken place during a birthday dinner, and the memory of what was said would be stored in association with that occasion. Just as the category of “city” might provide a retrieval cue for the semantic memory “Beijing”, the mention of “40th birthday” might act as a cue for the retrieval of what had been said over that dinner. The more strongly these autobiographical memories are associated with the time and circumstances of their occurrence, the greater their accessibility is likely to be.

   “Flashbulb memories”, which are stored when a highly memorable event – such as the 9/11 terrorist attacks – occurs, are an extreme example of this. Tulving described recollection from episodic memory as “mental time travel”, involving us in a revisiting of the past to access the memory. In his later work he pointed out that episodic memory is unique in featuring a subjective sense of time.

   Specific to humans, it involves not merely awareness of what has been, but also of what may come about. This unique ability allows us to reflect on our lives, worry about future events, and make plans. It is what enables humankind to “take full advantage of its awareness of its continued existence in time” and has allowed us to transform the natural world into one of numerous civilizations and cultures. Through this facility, “time’s arrow is bent into a loop”.

"Remembering is mental time travel."
                           Endel Tulving

Encoding information

   Tulving realized that organization is the key to efficient recall for both semantic and episodic memory, and that the brain somehow organizes information so that specific facts and events are “pigeonholed ” with related items. Recalling that specific information is then made easier by direction to the appropriate pigeonhole – the brain “knows where to look” for the memory it wants and can narrow down the search.

   The implication, he believed, is that the brain encodes each memory for storage in long-term memory, so that specific memories can be located for recollection by a more general retrieval cue. The cues that prompt episodic memory are usually sensory. A specific sound, such as a piece of music, or a scent can trigger a complete memory. Tulvings’s theory of the “encoding specificity principle” was especially applicable to episodic memory.

   Memories of specific past events are encoded according to the time of their occurrence, along with other memories of the same time. He found that the most effective cue for retrieving any specific episodic memory is the one which overlaps with it most, as this is stored together with the memory to be retrieved. Retrieval cues are necessary to access episodic memory, but not always sufficient, because sometimes the relationship is not close enough to allow recollection, even though the information is stored and available in long-term memory.

   Unlike previous theories of memory, Tulving’s encoding principle made a distinction between memory that is available and that which is accessible. When someone is unable to recall a piece of information, it does not mean that it is “forgotten” in the sense that it has faded or simply disappeared from long-term memory; it may still be stored, and therefore be available – the problem is one of retrieval.

image.png

Different types of memory are physically distinct, according to Tulving, because each behaves and functions in a significantly different way.

Scanning for memory

   Tulving’s research into the storage and retrieval of memory opened up a whole new area for psychological study. The publication of his findings in the 1970s coincided with a new determination by many cognitive psychologists to find confirmation of their theories in neuroscience, using brain-imaging techniques that had just become available.

   In conjunction with neuroscientists, Tulving was able to map the areas of the brain that are active during encoding and retrieval of memory, and establish that episodic memory is associated with the medial temporal lobe and, specifically, the hippocampus Partly due to his unorthodox and untutored-approach, Tulving made innovative insights that proved inspirational to other psychologists, including some of his former students such as Daniel Schacter.

   Tulving’s focus on storage and retrieval provided a new way of thinking about memory, but it was perhaps his distinction between semantic and episodic memory that was his breakthrough contribution. It allowed subsequent psychologists to increase the complexity of the model to include such concepts as procedural memory (remembering how to do something), and the difference between explicit memory (of which we are consciously aware) and implicit memory (of which we have no conscious awareness, but which nonetheless continues to affect us).

   These topics remain of great interest to cognitive psychologists today. Emotional events such as weddings give rise to episodic memories. These are stored in such a way that the person remembering relives the event, in a form of “time travel”.

MORE TO KNOW…

APPROACH

Memory studies

BEFORE

1878 Hermann Ebbinghaus conducts the first scientific study of human memory.
1927 Bluma Zeigarnik describes how interrupted tasks are better remembered than uninterrupted ones.
1960s Jerome Bruner stresses the importance of organization and categorization in the learning process.

AFTER

1979 Elizabeth Loftus looks at distortions of memory in her book Eyewitness Testimony.
1981 Gordon H. Bower makes the link between events and emotions in memory.
2001 Daniel Schacter publishes The Seven Sins of Memory: How the Mind Forgets and Remembers.

ENDEL TULVING

   Born the son of a judge in Tartu, Estonia, Endel Tulving was educated at a private school for boys, and although a model student, he was more interested in sports than academic subjects. When Russia invaded in 1944, he and his brother escaped to Germany to finish their studies and did not see their parents again until after the death of Stalin 9 years later. After World War II, Tulving worked as a translator for the American army and briefly attended medical school before emigrating to Canada in 1949.

   He was accepted as a student at the University of Toronto, where he graduated in psychology in 1953, and took his MA degree in 1954. He then moved to Harvard where he gained a PhD for his thesis on visual perception. In 1956, Tulving returned to the University of Toronto, where he continues to teach to this day.

Key works

1972 Organization of Memory
1983 Elements of Episodic Memory
1999 Memory, Consciousness, and the Brain

以下是我写的关于The Psychology Book的其他章节,欢迎各位前来观看 ^ ^

0.《Abstract & comment》—— Big Ideas Simply Explained
1.《The Introduction》—— Science Of mind And Behavior
2.《The new science》—— The science of mind and behavior
3.《The philosophical Root》—— psychology in the making
4.《The Four Temperaments》—— Earth, Air, Fire, Water
48.《Acquiring knowledge》——Process not Product
49.《Conviction Man》—— Hard Change
50. 《Memory Limits》—— Magical Number 7
51. 《The Cognitive Psychology》——Mental Processes
52. 《Attention theory》——One Voice One Time
53.《Memory》——Mental time travel
54.《Perception&Prospect theory》
55.《Memory》—— Events & Emotions
56.《Emotion》—— Runaway Train
57.《Flow》——State Of Ecstasy
58.《Life》—Good Social Relationship
最后编辑于
©著作权归作者所有,转载或内容合作请联系作者
  • 序言:七十年代末,一起剥皮案震惊了整个滨河市,随后出现的几起案子,更是在滨河造成了极大的恐慌,老刑警刘岩,带你破解...
    沈念sama阅读 218,546评论 6 507
  • 序言:滨河连续发生了三起死亡事件,死亡现场离奇诡异,居然都是意外死亡,警方通过查阅死者的电脑和手机,发现死者居然都...
    沈念sama阅读 93,224评论 3 395
  • 文/潘晓璐 我一进店门,熙熙楼的掌柜王于贵愁眉苦脸地迎上来,“玉大人,你说我怎么就摊上这事。” “怎么了?”我有些...
    开封第一讲书人阅读 164,911评论 0 354
  • 文/不坏的土叔 我叫张陵,是天一观的道长。 经常有香客问我,道长,这世上最难降的妖魔是什么? 我笑而不...
    开封第一讲书人阅读 58,737评论 1 294
  • 正文 为了忘掉前任,我火速办了婚礼,结果婚礼上,老公的妹妹穿的比我还像新娘。我一直安慰自己,他们只是感情好,可当我...
    茶点故事阅读 67,753评论 6 392
  • 文/花漫 我一把揭开白布。 她就那样静静地躺着,像睡着了一般。 火红的嫁衣衬着肌肤如雪。 梳的纹丝不乱的头发上,一...
    开封第一讲书人阅读 51,598评论 1 305
  • 那天,我揣着相机与录音,去河边找鬼。 笑死,一个胖子当着我的面吹牛,可吹牛的内容都是我干的。 我是一名探鬼主播,决...
    沈念sama阅读 40,338评论 3 418
  • 文/苍兰香墨 我猛地睁开眼,长吁一口气:“原来是场噩梦啊……” “哼!你这毒妇竟也来了?” 一声冷哼从身侧响起,我...
    开封第一讲书人阅读 39,249评论 0 276
  • 序言:老挝万荣一对情侣失踪,失踪者是张志新(化名)和其女友刘颖,没想到半个月后,有当地人在树林里发现了一具尸体,经...
    沈念sama阅读 45,696评论 1 314
  • 正文 独居荒郊野岭守林人离奇死亡,尸身上长有42处带血的脓包…… 初始之章·张勋 以下内容为张勋视角 年9月15日...
    茶点故事阅读 37,888评论 3 336
  • 正文 我和宋清朗相恋三年,在试婚纱的时候发现自己被绿了。 大学时的朋友给我发了我未婚夫和他白月光在一起吃饭的照片。...
    茶点故事阅读 40,013评论 1 348
  • 序言:一个原本活蹦乱跳的男人离奇死亡,死状恐怖,灵堂内的尸体忽然破棺而出,到底是诈尸还是另有隐情,我是刑警宁泽,带...
    沈念sama阅读 35,731评论 5 346
  • 正文 年R本政府宣布,位于F岛的核电站,受9级特大地震影响,放射性物质发生泄漏。R本人自食恶果不足惜,却给世界环境...
    茶点故事阅读 41,348评论 3 330
  • 文/蒙蒙 一、第九天 我趴在偏房一处隐蔽的房顶上张望。 院中可真热闹,春花似锦、人声如沸。这庄子的主人今日做“春日...
    开封第一讲书人阅读 31,929评论 0 22
  • 文/苍兰香墨 我抬头看了看天上的太阳。三九已至,却和暖如春,着一层夹袄步出监牢的瞬间,已是汗流浃背。 一阵脚步声响...
    开封第一讲书人阅读 33,048评论 1 270
  • 我被黑心中介骗来泰国打工, 没想到刚下飞机就差点儿被人妖公主榨干…… 1. 我叫王不留,地道东北人。 一个月前我还...
    沈念sama阅读 48,203评论 3 370
  • 正文 我出身青楼,却偏偏与公主长得像,于是被迫代替她去往敌国和亲。 传闻我的和亲对象是个残疾皇子,可洞房花烛夜当晚...
    茶点故事阅读 44,960评论 2 355

推荐阅读更多精彩内容

  • rljs by sennchi Timeline of History Part One The Cognitiv...
    sennchi阅读 7,332评论 0 10
  • **2014真题Directions:Read the following text. Choose the be...
    又是夜半惊坐起阅读 9,509评论 0 23
  • 昨晚梦见不知道什么好吃的,说了梦话,一堆好吃的。 总是说梦话,说过各种无数让人无语的梦中感慨。
    八十块两条阅读 161评论 0 0
  • 嗯,我离职了,我是再今年17年的四月一日辞职的,对,你们想象的每次,四月一日愚人节啊!不是愚人,而是每年的愚人节都...
    徐小磊家的沙莎阅读 145评论 0 0
  • 若此生你我相识,我愿陪你到天明。若此生你我相知,我愿陪你雨中走。若此生你我相爱,我愿陪你到白头。若此生你我相守,我...
    深秋一人看枫叶阅读 726评论 0 0